Biomecanicamente.org actualidad on-line del Instituto de Biomecánica ■ Biomecanicamente.org IBV on-line news ■ Biomecánica, ciencia que estudia el comportamiento del cuerpo humano y su relación con los productos con que interactúa y el entorno en el que se desenvuelve ■ Biomechanics, the study of the human body's behaviour and its relation to both its surrounding, environment and the products it interacts with ■ Éxito empresarial a través del bienestar de las personas ■ Corporate success through people's wellbeing ■ Cuidamos tu calidad de vida ■ We take care of your quality of life ■ Automoción y medios de transporte ■ Automotive and mass transport sector ■
Tuesday, 05 June 2018 09:53

Development of a virtual driver to analyze potential autonomous vehicle functions Featured

Rate this item
(1 Vote)

Juan Manuel Belda Lois*; Helios de Rosario Martínez; Javier Esteban Muñoz; Nicolás Palomares Olivares; José S. Solaz Sanahuja; Elisa Signes Pérez  

Instituto de Biomecánica (IBV) Universitat Politècnica de València. Edificio 9C. Camino de Vera s/n (46022) Valencia. Spain

* IBV’s Healthcare Technology Group, CIBER in Bioengineering, Biomaterials and Nanomedicine (CIBER-BBN)

 

Analyzing new on-board and driving assistance systems requires complex and costly tests. The possibility of analyzing the complexity and driver risks associated to the use of these systems from the early stages of the development is key to guarantee safety and lower development costs.

With this purpose, a cognitive model (boxes diagram that represents the thinking process of a person in certain tasks or activities) of a vehicle driver has been developed. The model includes the three different levels of decision required for properly driving: strategic, navigation and control. The model has been implemented as a Discrete Event Model and includes a model of Declarative Memory and a Model for Advanced Workload Analysis. Nowadays, a modified scenario of the Lane Change Test (LCT) has been already implemented and it is about to be validated with real users. In the coming months, the Model should will be able to drive itself the LCT in the simulation platform at Instituto de Biomecánica (IBV), emulating the behavior of real users.

INTRODUCTION

Driving is a complex and potentially dangerous task involving attention mechanisms and control actions at different cognitive (the process of thought) levels and different temporal scales: Drivers need to take a number of actions to arrive at their target place. Besides, these actions should be mediated by the state of the traffic and the environmental conditions. To achieve this, drivers must use cognitive tasks (level of thought process a person has reached or is using to solve a specific problem) involving strategic planning, but also sophisticated control and (human) motor coordination aspects.

Figure 1. Example of a driving situation.

The increase of vehicle automation and vehicle information systems (IVIS) introduce new constraints to the driving activity (Cacciabue 2007) that makes necessary to deep into cognitive modelling.

In order to tackle this complex scenario, there have been different approaches to model the behaviour of vehicle drivers that can be classified into (Engström and Hollnagel 2007):

♦ Taxonomic models: They are descriptive models or inventories of facts, which provide a description of the tasks related to driving without digging into the causes of those facts. Taxonomic models are built through task analysis, and are used to define the complexity of the activity that has been analysed, detect critical points, possible errors, etc. However, there is no possibility to ascertain what may really happen in different circumstances.

♦ Functional models: A key difference between taxonomic and functional models is that the latter connect components of the tasks that are done by the driver with time requirements and constraints. Functional models can define the dynamics of driver behaviour, and predict problems and decisions that are made by the drivers.

Functional models include the aforementioned cognitive models, in which mental processes of the driver determine what task is done in each moment, and the level of performance achieved by the human driver. There are sophisticated cognitive models, like the so-called “cognitive architectures”, which are potentially a powerful tool that can simulate mental processes, for a better analysis of the problems and risks that may arise during complex tasks. However such cognitive architectures have been seldom used in models of vehicle drivers with a few exceptions (Rosenbloom et al. 1991), (Salvucci 2006). The main drawback in the approaches based on cognitive architectures are the interaction of the different levels of control required for driving, while these architectures are mainly devoted to exploit just one of these levels.

A different approach involving functional simulation and modelling of human cognitive constraints is the “Improved Performance Research Integration Tool” (Kandemir, Handley, and Thompson n.d.) (IMPRINT) developed by the US Army Research Laboratory (ARL).

IMPRINT uses two approaches:

♦ For modelling purposes, IMPRINT uses a Discrete Event Model approach, in which any change in the context (the perception of something in the road or the vehicle, an action of the driver, a change in the motion of the vehicle, or anything that may happen) is defined as an “event”, and a driving scenario is a sequence of events in time. Some events may be predestined to happen (e.g. a traffic light goes red), or may be activated by the combination of past processes and some rules defined in the system.

♦ For cognitive components, IMPRINT uses the Advanced Workload Analysis (Mitchell 2000), which is mainly devoted to the estimation of workload. In that approach, all processes of perception, reasoning, decision or action involved in a task, interact with each other such that using the cognitive resources of a person and the demands of the task components, an amount of “cognitive workload” (level of measurable mental effort put forth by an individual in response to one or more cognitive tasks) is calculated. Such models can determine the evolution of the driver’s workload in a scenario and the response times due to the processing of the workload made by the driver.

We have used this approach for developing a model of vehicle driver, improving the model taking into consideration the two lower levels involved in driving: The control of the vehicle and the motor control (control of body movements).

MODEL DEVELOPMENT

The tasks of the driver (e.g. move to another lane of the road, change the driving mode, look at the road) are coded as “processes”, which are sequences of events that activate or interrupt other processes and events, and are accompanied by calculations that modify the environment, for instance the position and dynamics of the vehicle, signs that appear at the sight of the driver, etc. Those processes operate at many levels, such that complex tasks are defined in a modular fashion: at the lowest level, there are “timeout” processes whose only effect is the pass of a defined amount of time. Building upon those “timeouts”, cognitive actions or basic motor control tasks with fixed time demands are defined, like looking at the road, recognising a sign, steering the wheel, etc. More complex tasks like vehicle control, lane change, switch driving mode, etc., are modelled by combinations or repetitions of those atomic tasks, such that the time that they take and their outcome may be affected by the interaction between the many processes and sub-processes that are running at the same time.

The model includes the following components (Figure 2):

♦ The elements from the environment constraining the decision of the driver (other vehicles, the road, the IVIS, signals, etc.).

♦ A model for Advanced Workload Analysis.

♦ A learning model to adjust task performance adaptively.

Figure 2. Chart with the components for cognitive driver simulation. The boxes with the white strip are the components of the programming language. The boxes with light blue strip are components for the environmental control model. The boxes with dark blue strip are  he components of the declarative memory simulation.The boxes with the green  strip are the components for the Advanced Workload Analysis.

The Advanced Workload Analysis has been modelled with the workload estimations taken from literature (Bierbaum, Szabo, and Aldrich 1989). In that model, the cognitive tasks of the driver are special processes with a predefined completion time, which also consume a variable amount of work capacity, depending on the amount and type of concurrent tasks. A set of rules for prioritising and interrupting cognitive tasks simulate the delays in task completion due to the difficulty of making several conflicting tasks at the same time. Each time a new task is started, there is an evaluation, and, depending on their priority and available load, they can be done, delayed, or a lower priority task can be interrupted and left for resume later.

The learning model is based on the concept of “declarative memory”, a cognitive architecture used in Artificial Intelligence (Anderson, Matessa, and Lebiere 1997). This model adapts the time that is defined for the completion of cognitive tasks, on the basis of the number of times and how recently those tasks have been done or recalled in the near past.

The model has been applied to simulate a modified version of the Lane Change Test (LCT) as described in ISO 26022:2010 (en). This simulation involves the three levels of control required for driving:

♦ Strategic control is determined by the Lane Change Test, and consists in driving straight for 3 km.

♦ Navigation, it is simulated by the Advanced Workload Analysis and takes into consideration the processes of decision with the time required to do it.

♦ Motor control, it is simulated by the declarative memory model and the model of turning the steering wheel by the model of Salvucci (2006).

SIMULATION SCENARIO

The modified Lane Change Test simulates a driver with an autonomous vehicle taking the control of the car and recovering the control after autonomous driving while having to manage a proper lane change according to the signals present in the environment (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Graphical representation of the driving states of the model: The simulation emulates the transition from AV to manual (left figure).

In this scenario, the decision of the lane change is mediated by the decision of executing the commands related to the autonomous system.

RESULTS

100 simulations of the cognitive model have been run considering secondary tasks (change from manual to automatic and from automatic to manual) and 100 simulations without considering the secondary tasks. The 100 simulations have been executed on the Lane Change Test (LCT) test on a 3000 meter circuit in the same simulated circuit. The same circuit has been used in all tests in order to make comparisons. For the simulations with secondary task, the events of change from automatic to manual and manual to automatic have been added in the same positions.

Figure 4. Comparison of the trajectories of the cognitive model driving simulations with (in red) and without secondary task (in green). Delays in the decision to turn when there is cognitive workload provoked by the secondary task can be observed,

The results of both models show (Figure 4) a greater variability at the moment of turning and delays in the decision to turn. The delay is clearer when there are several concurrent tasks (see x coordinate equal to 1400 in the figure).

CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE WORK

A cognitive model has been implemented in a Discrete Event Approach. This model is flexible to be adapted to a wide range of real world scenarios. Nowadays a modified version of the Lane Change Test has been simulated. The results from the test are about to be validated with real users.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The present work has been developed under the frame of the project DIVEO.

DIVEO project is financed by the IVACE within the framework of the program of grants directed to technological centers of the Valencian Community for the development of non-economic R & D projects carried out in cooperation with companies for the financial year 2017, co-financed by the European Development Fund Regional (ERDF) in a percentage of 50% through the ERDF Operational Program of the Comunitat Valenciana 2014-2020

REFERENCES

Anderson, John R., Michael Matessa, and Christian Lebiere. 1997. ‘ACT-R: A Theory of Higher Level Cognition and Its Relation to Visual Attention’. Human-Computer Interaction 12 (4):439–462.

 

Bierbaum, Carl R., Sandra M. Szabo, and Theodore B. Aldrich. 1989. ‘Task Analysis of the UH-60 Mission and Decision Rules for Developing a UH-60 Workload Prediction Model. Volume 1. Summary Report’. ANACAPA SCIENCES INC FORT RUCKER AL. http://www.dtic.mil/docs/citations/ADA210763.

 

Cacciabue, Carlo, ed. 2007. Modelling Driver Behaviour in Automotive Environments: Critical Issues in Driver Interactions with Intelligent Transport Systems. 2007 edition. London: Springer.

 

Engström, Johan, and Erik Hollnagel. 2007. ‘A General Conceptual Framework for Modelling Behavioural Effects of Driver Support Functions’. In Modelling Driver Behaviour in Automotive Environments, 61–84. Springer. http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-1-84628-618-6_4.

 

Kandemir, Cansu, Holly A. H. Handley, and Deborah Thompson. n.d. ‘A Workload Model to Evaluate Distracters and Driver’s Aids’. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics. Accessed 22 September 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ergon.2016.09.004.

 

Mitchell, Diane K. 2000. ‘Mental Workload and ARL Workload Modeling Tools’. DTIC Document. http://oai.dtic.mil/oai/oai?verb=getRecord&metadataPrefix=html&identifier=ADA377300.

 

Rosenbloom, Paul S., John E. Laird, Allen Newell, and Robert McCarl. 1991. ‘A Preliminary Analysis of the Soar Architecture as a Basis for General Intelligence’. Artificial Intelligence 47 (1):289–325.

 

Salvucci, Dario D. 2006. ‘Modeling Driver Behavior in a Cognitive Architecture’. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 48 (2):362–380.

 

Wickens, Christopher D. 2002. ‘Multiple Resources and Performance Prediction’. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science 3 (2):159–177.

 

Read 4045 times Last modified on Thursday, 07 June 2018 12:14



PROXIMAMENTE / COMING SOON

BUSCAR / FIND

SOCIAL MEDIA

facebook logo Twitter-X logo YouTube logo

NOSOTROS / ABOUT

 

INSTITUTO DE BIOMECÁNICA (IBV)

Universitat Politècnica de València •  Edificio 9C

Camino de Vera s/n  •  E-46022 Valencia  •  Spain

Tel. +34 96 111 11 70 •  +34 610 567 200

comunicacion@ibv.org  •  www.ibv.org

Copyright © 2024 Instituto de Biomecánica